The role of object ownership on online inhibition in peripersonal space.

Item request has been placed! ×
Item request cannot be made. ×
loading   Processing Request
  • Additional Information
    • Source:
      Publisher: Blackwell Country of Publication: United States NLM ID: 0142657 Publication Model: Print-Electronic Cited Medium: Internet ISSN: 1469-8986 (Electronic) Linking ISSN: 00485772 NLM ISO Abbreviation: Psychophysiology Subsets: MEDLINE
    • Publication Information:
      Publication: Malden, MA : Blackwell
      Original Publication: Baltimore, Williams & Wilkins.
    • Subject Terms:
    • Abstract:
      Peripersonal space (PPS), as opposed to extrapersonal space (EPS), refers to the area surrounding the body within which individuals interact with objects or conspecifics. However, objects in PPS can belong to oneself or to others, which was found to influence how these objects are encoded. We analyzed the performances of motor responses in a reachability judgment task concerning self-owned and other-owned objects (cups) presented in PPS or EPS. EMG activities were recorded on the thumbs (flexor pollicis brevis) to detect correct and erroneous motor activations. Behavioral data showed that motor responses were shorter and longer for self-owned cups compared to other-owned cups in PPS and EPS, respectively. Ten percent of trials showed initial response errors, which were higher in the EPS for self-owned cups and in the PPS for other-owned cups. Eighty-two percent of these errors were corrected online, with corrections being more efficient for self-owned cups in the PPS. Overall, the data revealed that reachability judgments were faster and more accurate in the PPS, with more efficient inhibition processes in the presence of motor errors. Motor selection and correction are thus modulated by the social context of object ownership, highlighting the specific role of the PPS in encoding self-relevant objects for action.
      (© 2024 Society for Psychophysiological Research.)
    • References:
      Aron, A. R. (2007). The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive control. The Neuroscientist, 13(3), 214–228.
      Banich, M. T., & Depue, B. E. (2015). Recent advances in understanding neural systems that support inhibitory control. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 1, 17–22.
      Barkley, R. A. (2012). Executive functions: What they are, how they work, and why they evolved. Guilford Press.
      Bartolo, A., Coello, Y., Edwards, M. G., Delepoulle, S., Endo, S., & Wing, A. M. (2014). Contribution of the motor system to the perception of reachable space: An fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 40(12), 3807–3817.
      Beggan, J. K. (1992). On the social nature of nonsocial perception: The mere ownership effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(2), 229–237.
      Bestmann, S., & Duque, J. (2016). Transcranial magnetic stimulation: Decomposing the processes underlying action preparation. The Neuroscientist, 22(4), 392–405.
      Blini, E., Desoche, C., Salemme, R., Kabil, A., Hadj‐Bouziane, F., & Farnè, A. (2018). Mind the depth: Visual perception of shapes is better in peripersonal space. Psychological Science, 29(11), 1868–1877.
      Bonferroni, C. E. (1936). Teoria statistica delle classi e calcolo delle probabilita. Pubblicazioni del R Istituto Superiore di Scienze Economiche e Commerciali di Firenze, 8, 3–62.
      Bouisset, S., & Maton, B. (1972). Quantitative relationship between surface EMG and intramuscular electromyographic activity in voluntary movement. American Journal of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 51(6), 285–295.
      Bourgeois, J., & Coello, Y. (2012). Effect of visuomotor calibration and uncertainty on the perception of peripersonal space. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1268–1283.
      Bourgeois, J., Farnè, A., & Coello, Y. (2014). Costs and benefits of tool‐use on the perception of reachable space. Acta Psychologica, 148, 91–95.
      Box, G. E. P., & Cox, D. R. (1964). An analysis of transformations. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B, 26, 211–252.
      Brozzoli, C., Makin, T. R., Cardinali, L., Holmes, N. P., & Farnè, A. (2011). Peripersonal space: A multisensory interface for body–object interactions. In M. M. Murray & M. T. Wallace (Eds.), The neural bases of multisensory processes. CRC Press/Taylor & Francis.
      Burle, B., Possamaï, C. A., Vidal, F., Bonnet, M., & Hasbroucq, T. (2002). Executive control in the Simon effect: An electromyographic and distributional analysis. Psychological Research, 66, 324–336.
      Burle, B., Roger, C., Allain, S., Vidal, F., & Hasbroucq, T. (2008). Error negativity does not reflect conflict: A reappraisal of conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex activity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(9), 1637–1655.
      Cardellicchio, P., Sinigaglia, C., & Costantini, M. (2011). The space of affordances: A TMS study. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 1369–1372.
      Cartaud, A., & Coello, Y. (2020). ATHOS: a dATabase of 48 3D Human virtual characters with non‐emOtional facial expreSsion for virtual reality.
      Chao, L. L., & Martin, A. (2000). Representation of manipulable man‐made objects in the dorsal stream. NeuroImage, 12(4), 478–484.
      Cléry, J., Guipponi, O., Odouard, S., Wardak, C., & Hamed, S. B. (2018). Cortical networks for encoding near and far space in the non‐human primate. NeuroImage, 176, 164–178.
      Coello, Y., & Bonnotte, I. (2013). The mutual roles of action representations and spatial Deictics in French language. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(11), 2187–2203.
      Coello, Y., & Cartaud, A. (2021). The interrelation between peripersonal action space and interpersonal social space: Psychophysiological evidence and clinical implications. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 15, 636124.
      Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97(3), 332.
      Colby, C. L., Duhamel, J. R., & Goldberg, M. E. (1993). Ventral intraparietal area of the macaque: Anatomic location and visual response properties. Journal of Neurophysiology, 69(3), 902–914.
      Coles, M. G. H., Gratton, G., Bashore, T. R., Eriksen, C. W., & Donchin, E. (1985). A psychophysiological investigation of the continuous flow of human information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 529–552.
      Constable, M. D., Kritikos, A., & Bayliss, A. P. (2011). Grasping the concept of personal property. Cognition, 119(3), 430–437.
      Costantini, M., Ambrosini, E., Tieri, G., Sinigaglia, C., & Committeri, G. (2010). Where does an object trigger an action? An investigation about affordances in space. Experimental Brain Research, 207, 95–103.
      Coxon, J. P., Stinear, C. M., & Byblow, W. D. (2006). Intracortical inhibition during volitional inhibition of prepared action. Journal of Neurophysiology, 95(6), 3371–3383.
      Culham, J. C., Gallivan, J., Cavina‐Pratesi, C., & Quinlan, D. J. (2008). fMRI investigations of reaching and ego space in human superior parieto‐occipital cortex. In R. L. Klatzky, B. MacWhinney, & M. Behrman (Eds.), Embodiment, ego‐space, and action (pp. 247–274). Psychology Press.
      Cunningham, S. J., Turk, D. J., Macdonald, L. M., & Macrae, C. N. (2008). Yours or mine? Ownership and memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(1), 312–318.
      de Vignemont, F., & Iannetti, G. D. (2015). How many peripersonal spaces? Neuropsychologia, 70, 327–334.
      Derosiere, G., & Duque, J. (2020). Tuning the corticospinal system: How distributed brain circuits shape human actions. The Neuroscientist, 26(4), 359–379.
      Dienes, Z. (2014). Using Bayes to get the most out of non‐significant results. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 781.
      Dufour, A., & Touzalin, P. (2008). Improved visual sensitivity in the perihand space. Experimental Brain Research, 190, 91–98.
      El‐Sourani, N., Wurm, M. F., Trempler, I., Fink, G. R., & Schubotz, R. I. (2018). Making sense of objects lying around: How contextual objects shape brain activity during action observation. NeuroImage, 167, 429–437.
      Ferri, F., Costantini, M., Huang, Z., Perrucci, M. G., Ferretti, A., Romani, G. L., & Northoff, G. (2015). Intertrial variability in the premotor cortex accounts for individual differences in peripersonal space. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(50), 16328–16339.
      Fox, J., & Weisberg, S. (2019). An R companion to applied regression (Third ed.). Sage. https://socialsciences.mcmaster.ca/jfox/Books/Companion/.
      Friedman, M. (1937). The use of ranks to avoid the assumption of normality implicit in the analysis of variance. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 32(200), 675–701.
      Gigliotti, M. F., Soares Coelho, P., Coutinho, J., & Coello, Y. (2019). Peripersonal space in social context is modulated by action reward, but differently in males and females. Psychological Research, 85, 181–194.
      Golubickis, M., & Macrae, C. N. (2021). That's me in the spotlight: Self‐relevance modulates attentional breadth. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 28(6), 1915–1922.
      Gori, M., Sciutti, A., Burr, D., & Sandini, G. (2011). Direct and indirect haptic calibration of visual size judgments. PLoS One, 6(10), e25599.
      Grafton, S. T., Fadiga, L., Arbib, M. A., & Rizzolatti, G. (1997). Premotor cortex activation during observation and naming of familiar tools. NeuroImage, 6(4), 231–236.
      Graziano, M. S., & Cooke, D. F. (2006). Parieto‐frontal interactions, personal space, and defensive behavior. Neuropsychologia, 44(13), 2621–2635.
      Graziano, M. S., & Gross, C. G. (1993). A bimodal map of space: Somatosensory receptive fields in the macaque putamen with corresponding visual receptive fields. Experimental Brain Research, 97, 96–109.
      Graziano, M. S., Yap, G. S., & Gross, C. G. (1994). Coding of visual space by premotor neurons. Science, 266(5187), 1054–1057.
      Grisetto, F., Delevoye‐Turrell, Y. N., & Roger, C. (2019). Efficient but less active monitoring system in individuals with high aggressive predispositions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 146, 125–132.
      Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F., & Rangel, A. (2009). Self‐control in decision‐making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science, 324(5927), 646–648.
      Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F., Knoepfle, D. T., O'Doherty, J. P., & Rangel, A. (2010). Value computations in ventral medial prefrontal cortex during charitable decision making incorporate input from regions involved in social cognition. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(2), 583–590.
      Hasbroucq, T., Possamaï, C.‐A., Bonnet, M., & Vidal, F. (1999). Effect of the irrelevant location of the response signal on choice reaction time: An electromyographic study in humans. Psychophysiology, 36, 522–526.
      Hasbroucq, T., Tandonnet, C., Micallef‐Roll, J., Blin, O., & Possamaï, C. A. (2003). An electromyographic analysis of the effect of levodopa on the response time of healthy subjects. Psychopharmacology, 165, 313–316.
      Hofmann, W., & Kotabe, H. (2012). A general model of preventive and interventive self‐control. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 6(10), 707–722.
      Iachini, T., Coello, Y., Frassinetti, F., Senese, V. P., Galante, F., & Ruggiero, G. (2016). Peripersonal and interpersonal space in virtual and real environments: Effects of gender and age. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 45, 154–164.
      Jeffreys, H. (1961). Theory of probability (3rd ed.). Oxford University Press.
      Klawohn, J., Meyer, A., Weinberg, A., & Hajcak, G. (2020). Methodological choices in event‐related potential (ERP) research and their impact on internal consistency reliability and individual differences: An examination of the error‐related negativity (ERN) and anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 129(1), 29.
      Leclere, N. X., Sarlegna, F. R., Coello, Y., & Bourdin, C. (2019). Sensori‐motor adaptation to novel limb dynamics influences the representation of peripersonal space. Neuropsychologia, 131, 193–204.
      Lenglart, L., Cartaud, A., Quesque, F., Sampaio, A., & Coello, Y. (2022). Object coding in peripersonal space depends on object ownership. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76(8), 1925–1939.
      Leonard, C. T., Brown, J. S., Price, T. R., Queen, S. A., & Mikhailenok, E. L. (2004). Comparison of surface electromyography and myotonometric measurements during voluntary isometric contractions. Journal of Electromyography and Kinesiology, 14(6), 709–714.
      Liesefeld, H. R. (2018). Estimating the timing of cognitive operations with MEG/EEG latency measures: A primer, a brief tutorial, and an implementation of various methods. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 12, 765.
      Liu, M., He, X., Rotsthein, P., & Sui, J. (2016). Dynamically orienting your own face facilitates the automatic attraction of attention. Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(1–4), 37–44.
      Lockwood, P. L., Wittmann, M. K., Apps, M. A., Klein‐Flügge, M. C., Crockett, M. J., Humphreys, G. W., & Rushworth, M. F. (2018). Neural mechanisms for learning self and other ownership. Nature Communications, 9(1), 4747.
      Luck, S. J., Hillyard, S. A., Mouloua, M., Woldorff, M. G., Clark, V. P., & Hawkins, H. L. (1994). Effects of spatial cuing on luminance detectability: Psychophysical and electrophysiological evidence for early selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(4), 887.
      Mackenzie, I. G., & Dudschig, C. (2021). DMCfun: An R package for fitting Diffusion Model of Conflict (DMC) to reaction time and error rate data. Methods in Psychology, 5, 100074.
      Morewedge, C. K. (2021). Psychological ownership: Implicit and explicit. Current Opinion in Psychology, 39, 125–132.
      Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.
      Parkinson, J., & Haggard, P. (2015). Choosing to stop: Responses evoked by externally triggered and internally generated inhibition identify a neural mechanism of will. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(10), 1948–1956.
      Patané, I., Brozzoli, C., Koun, E., Frassinetti, F., & Farnè, A. (2021). Me, you, and our object: Peripersonal space recruitment during executed and observed actions depends on object ownership. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(7), 1410.
      Pfurtscheller, G., & Neuper, C. (1994). Event‐related synchronization of mu rhythm in the EEG over the cortical hand area in man. Neuroscience Letters, 174(1), 93–96.
      Proverbio, A. M. (2012). Tool perception suppresses 10–12 Hz μ rhythm of EEG over the somatosensory area. Biological Psychology, 91(1), 1–7.
      Quinlan, D. J., & Culham, J. C. (2007). fMRI reveals a preference for near viewing in the human parieto‐occipital cortex. NeuroImage, 36(1), 167–187.
      R Core Team. (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R‐project.org/.
      Raud, L., Thunberg, C., & Huster, R. J. (2022). Partial response electromyography as a marker of action stopping. eLife, 11, e70332.
      Reed, C. L., Grubb, J. D., & Steele, C. (2006). Hands up: Attentional prioritization of space near the hand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(1), 166.
      Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002a). Activation and suppression in conflict tasks: Empirical clarification through distributional analyses. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Attention & performance: Vol. 19. Common mechanisms in perception and action (pp. 494–519). Oxford University Press.
      Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002b). Micro‐ and macro‐adjustments of task set: Activation and suppression in conflict tasks. Psychological Research, 66, 312–323.
      Ridderinkhof, K. R., Forstmann, B. U., Wylie, S. A., Burle, B., & van den Wildenberg, W. P. (2011). Neurocognitive mechanisms of action control: Resisting the call of the sirens. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2(2), 174–192.
      Ripley, B., Venables, B., Bates, D. M., Hornik, K., Gebhardt, A., Firth, D., & Ripley, M. B. (2013). Package ‘mass’. Cran R, 538, 113–120.
      Rizzolatti, G., Scandolara, C., Matelli, M., & Gentilucci, M. (1981). Afferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. II. Visual responses. Behavioural Brain Research, 2(2), 147–163.
      Roger, C., Núñez Castellar, E., Pourtois, G., & Fias, W. (2014). Changing your mind before it is too late: The electrophysiological correlates of online error correction during response selection. Psychophysiology, 51(8), 746–760.
      Rosenfeld, P. J., & Olson, J. M. (2021). Bayesian data analysis: A fresh approach to power issues and null hypothesis interpretation. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 46, 135–140.
      Rouder, J. N., Speckman, P. L., Sun, D., Morey, R. D., & Iverson, G. (2009). Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 225–237.
      Sagaspe, P., Schwartz, S., & Vuilleumier, P. (2011). Fear and stop: A role for the amygdala in motor inhibition by emotional signals. NeuroImage, 55(4), 1825–1835.
      Schel, M. A., Kühn, S., Brass, M., Haggard, P., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & Crone, E. A. (2014). Neural correlates of intentional and stimulus‐driven inhibition: A comparison. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 27.
      Scheller, M., & Sui, J. (2022). The power of the self: Anchoring information processing across contexts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 48(9), 1001–1021.
      Schäfer, S., Wentura, D., & Frings, C. (2020). Creating a network of importance: The particular effects of self‐relevance on stimulus processing. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82, 3750–3766.
      Scorolli, C., Borghi, A. M., & Tummolini, L. (2018). Cues of control modulate the ascription of object ownership. Psychological Research, 82(5), 929–954.
      Sherratt, T. N., & Mesterton‐Gibbons, M. (2015). The evolution of respect for property. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 28(6), 1185–1202.
      Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory SR compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51(3), 300.
      Spence, C., Pavani, F., Maravita, A., & Holmes, N. (2004). Multisensory contributions to the 3‐D representation of visuotactile peripersonal space in humans: Evidence from the crossmodal congruency task. Journal of Physiology‐Paris, 98(1–3), 171–189.
      Staude, G., Flachenecker, C., Daumer, M., & Wolf, W. (2001). Onset detection in surface electromyographic signals: A systematic comparison of methods. EURASIP Journal on Advances in Signal Processing, 2001(2), 1–15.
      Suarez, I., Burle, B., Tobon, C., Pineda, D., Lopera, F., Hasbroucq, T., & Casini, L. (2015). Deciphering interference control in adults with ADHD by using distribution analyses and electromyographic activity. Acta Psychologica, 159, 85–92.
      Sui, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (2015). The integrative self: How self‐reference integrates perception and memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(12), 719–728.
      Sun, Y., Fuentes, L. J., Humphreys, G. W., & Sui, J. (2016). Try to see it my way: Domain‐specific embodiment enhances self and friend‐biases in perceptual matching. Cognition, 153, 108–117.
      Svensson, S., Golubickis, M., Johnson, S., Falbén, J. K., & Macrae, C. N. (2023). Self‐relevance and the activation of attentional networks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76(5), 1120–1130.
      Swick, D., Ashley, V., & Turken, U. (2011). Are the neural correlates of stopping and not going identical? Quantitative meta‐analysis of two response inhibition tasks. NeuroImage, 56(3), 1655–1665.
      Symons, C. S., & Johnson, B. T. (1997). The self‐reference effect in memory: A meta‐analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 121(3), 371.
      Tanenbaum, J., El‐Nasr, M. S., & Nixon, M. (2014). Nonverbal communication in virtual worlds. ETC Press.
      ter Horst, A. C., Van Lier, R., & Steenbergen, B. (2011). Spatial dependency of action simulation. Experimental Brain Research, 212, 635–644.
      Todd, R. M., & Manaligod, M. G. (2018). Implicit guidance of attention: The priority state space framework. Cortex, 102, 121–138.
      Toussaint, L., Wamain, Y., Bidet‐Ildei, C., & Coello, Y. (2020). Short‐term upper‐limb immobilization alters peripersonal space representation. Psychological Research, 84, 907–914.
      Turk, D. J., Van Bussel, K., Waiter, G. D., & Macrae, C. N. (2011). Mine and me: Exploring the neural basis of object ownership. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3657–3668.
      Ulrich, R., Schröter, H., Leuthold, H., & Birngruber, T. (2015). Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions. Cognitive Psychology, 78, 148–174.
      van Boxtel, G. J. M., Geraats, L. H. D., Van den Berg‐Lenssen, M. M. C., & Brunia, C. H. M. (1993). Detection of EMG onset in ERP research. Psychophysiology, 30(4), 405–412.
      Vincent, S. B. (1912). The function of the viborissae in the behavior of the white rat. Animal Behavior Monographs, 1(5), 1–82.
      Wamain, Y., Gabrielli, F., & Coello, Y. (2016). EEG μ rhythm in virtual reality reveals that motor coding of visual objects in peripersonal space is task dependent. Cortex, 74, 20–30.
      Wilcoxon, F. (1945). Individual comparisons by ranking methods. Biometrics Bulletin, 1(6), 80–83.
      Wolpert, D. M., & Landy, M. S. (2012). Motor control is decision‐making. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 22(6), 996–1003.
      World Medical Association. (2013). World medical association declaration of Helsinki: Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA, 310(20), 2191–2194.
      Ye, Y., & Gawronski, B. (2016). When possessions become part of the self: Ownership and implicit self‐object linking. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 64, 72–87.
      Yu, J., Tseng, P., Hung, D. L., Wu, S. W., & Juan, C. H. (2015). Brain stimulation improves cognitive control by modulating medial‐frontal activity and preSMA‐vmPFC functional connectivity. Human Brain Mapping, 36(10), 4004–4015.
      Zhang, R., Geng, X., & Lee, T. M. (2017). Large‐scale functional neural network correlates of response inhibition: An fMRI meta‐analysis. Brain Structure and Function, 222, 3973–3990.
    • Grant Information:
      ANR-21-ESRE-0030 Continuum Agence Nationale de la Recherche
    • Contributed Indexing:
      Keywords: EMG; Peripersonal space; motor inhibition; object ownership; partial error
    • Publication Date:
      Date Created: 20240729 Date Completed: 20240930 Latest Revision: 20240930
    • Publication Date:
      20240930
    • Accession Number:
      10.1111/psyp.14659
    • Accession Number:
      39072809