Sleep-dependent memory consolidation of televised content in infants.

Item request has been placed! ×
Item request cannot be made. ×
loading   Processing Request
  • Additional Information
    • Source:
      Publisher: Published on behalf of the European Sleep Research Society by Blackwell Scientific Publications Country of Publication: England NLM ID: 9214441 Publication Model: Print-Electronic Cited Medium: Internet ISSN: 1365-2869 (Electronic) Linking ISSN: 09621105 NLM ISO Abbreviation: J Sleep Res Subsets: MEDLINE
    • Publication Information:
      Original Publication: Oxford ; Boston : Published on behalf of the European Sleep Research Society by Blackwell Scientific Publications, c1992-
    • Subject Terms:
    • Abstract:
      Infants face the constant challenge of selecting information for encoding and storage from a continuous incoming stream of data. Sleep might help in this process by selectively consolidating new memory traces that are likely to be of future relevance. Using a deferred imitation paradigm and an experimental design, we asked whether 15- and 24-month-old infants (N = 105) who slept soon after encoding a televised demonstration of target actions would show higher imitation scores (retention) after a 24-h delay than same-aged infants who stayed awake for ≥4 h after encoding. In light of infants' well-known difficulties in learning and remembering information from screens, we tested if increasing the relevance of the televised content via standardised caregiver verbalisations might yield the highest imitation scores in the sleep condition. Regardless of sleep condition, 24-month-olds exhibited retention of target actions while 15-month-olds consistently failed to do so. For 24-month-olds, temporal recall was facilitated by sleep, but not by parental verbalisations. Correlational analyses revealed that more time asleep within 4 h after encoding was associated with better retention of the target actions and their temporal order in 24-months-olds. These results suggest that sleep facilitates memory consolidation of screen-based content in late infancy and that this effect might not hinge on caregivers' verbal engagement during viewing.
      (© 2023 The Authors. Journal of Sleep Research published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of European Sleep Research Society.)
    • References:
      Ambulatory Monitoring. (2023). Action‐W (Version 2.7.3285) [Computer Software]. Ambulatory Monitoring, Inc. https://www.ambulatory-monitoring.com/.
      Barr, R. (2013). Memory constraints on infant learning from picture books, television, and touchscreens. Child Development Perspectives, 7(4), 205–210. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12041.
      Barr, R. (2019). Growing up in the digital age: Early learning and family media ecology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(4), 341–346. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419838245.
      Barr, R., & Hayne, H. (1999). Developmental changes in imitation from television during infancy. Child Development, 70(5), 1067–1081. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00079.
      Barr, R., Kirkorian, H., Radesky, J., Coyne, S., Nichols, D., Blanchfield, O., Rusnak, S., Stockdale, L., Ribner, A., Durnez, J., Epstein, M., Heimann, M., Koch, F.‐S., Sundqvist, A., Birberg‐Thornberg, U., Konrad, C., Slussareff, M., Bus, A., Bellagamba, F., & Fitzpatrick, C. (2020). Beyond screen time: A synergistic approach to a more comprehensive assessment of family media exposure during early childhood. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, Article 1283. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01283.
      Barr, R., Muentener, P., Garcia, A., Fujimoto, M., & Chávez, V. (2007). The effect of repetition on imitation from television during infancy. Developmental Psychobiology, 49(2), 196–207. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20208.
      Barr, R., & Wyss, N. (2008). Reenactment of televised content by 2‐year olds: Toddlers use language learned from television to solve a difficult imitation problem. Infant Behavior & Development, 31(4), 696–703. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.04.006.
      Bauer, P. J., & Mandler, J. M. (1992). Putting the horse before the cart: The use of temporal order in recall of events by one‐year‐old children. Developmental Psychology, 28(3), 441–452. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.28.3.441.
      Cordi, M. J., & Rasch, B. (2021). How robust are sleep‐mediated memory benefits? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 67, 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2020.06.002.
      Demers, L. B., Hanson, K. G., Kirkorian, H. L., Pempek, T. A., & Anderson, D. R. (2013). Infant gaze following during parent‐infant coviewing of baby videos. Child Development, 84(2), 591–603. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01868.x.
      Diekelmann, S., & Born, J. (2010). The memory function of sleep. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(2), 114–126. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2762.
      Ewin, C. A., Reupert, A. E., McLean, L. A., & Ewin, C. J. (2021). The impact of joint media engagement on parent–child interactions: A systematic review. Human Behavior and Emerging Technologies, 3(2), 230–254. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbe2.203.
      Fischer, S., & Born, J. (2009). Anticipated reward enhances offline learning during sleep. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(6), 1586–1593. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017256.
      Galland, B., Meredith‐Jones, K., Gray, A., Sayers, R., Lawrence, J., Taylor, B., & Taylor, R. (2016). Criteria for nap identification in infants and young children using 24‐h actigraphy and agreement with parental diary. Sleep Medicine, 19, 85–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2015.10.013.
      Harris, P. L., & Corriveau, K. H. (2011). Young children's selective trust in informants. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1179–1187. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0321.
      Hayne, H., & Herbert, J. (2004). Verbal cues facilitate memory retrieval during infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89(2), 127–139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2004.06.002.
      Herbert, J., & Hayne, H. (2000). The ontogeny of long‐term retention during the second year of life. Developmental Science, 3(1), 50–56. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00099.
      Horváth, K., Hannon, B., Ujma, P. P., Gombos, F., & Plunkett, K. (2018). Memory in 3‐month‐old infants benefits from a short nap. Developmental Science, 21(3), e12587. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12587.
      Hupbach, A., Gomez, R. L., Bootzin, R. R., & Nadel, L. (2009). Nap‐dependent learning in infants. Developmental Science, 12(6), 1007–1012. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00837.x.
      Klinzing, J. G., Niethard, N., & Born, J. (2019). Mechanisms of systems memory consolidation during sleep. Nature Neuroscience, 22(10), 1598–1610. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0467-3.
      Konrad, C., Dirks, N. D., Warmuth, A., Herbert, J. S., Schneider, S., & Seehagen, S. (2019). Sleep‐dependent selective imitation in infants. Journal of Sleep Research, 28(1), e12777. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12777.
      Konrad, C., Seehagen, S., Schneider, S., & Herbert, J. S. (2016). Naps promote flexible memory retrieval in 12‐month‐old infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 58(7), 866–874. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21431.
      Mason, G. M., & Spencer, R. M. (2022). Sleep and memory in infancy and childhood. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 4, 89–108. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121020-033411.
      Meltzoff, A. N. (1985). Immediate and deferred imitation in fourteen‐ and twenty‐four‐month‐old infants. Child Development, 56(1), 62. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130174.
      Mindell, J. A., Gould, R. A., Tikotzy, L., Leichman, E. S., & Walters, R. M. (2019). Norm‐referenced scoring system for the brief infant sleep questionnaire ‐ revised (BISQ‐R). Sleep Medicine, 63, 106–114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2019.05.010.
      Myers, L. J., Crawford, E., Murphy, C., Aka‐Ezoua, E., & Felix, C. (2018). Eyes in the room trump eyes on the screen: Effects of a responsive co‐viewer on toddlers' responses to and learning from video chat. Journal of Children and Media, 12(3), 275–294. https://doi.org/10.1080/17482798.2018.1425889.
      Sadeh, A. (2004). A brief screening questionnaire for infant sleep problems: Validation and findings for an internet sample. Pediatrics, 113(6), e570–e577. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.113.6.e570.
      Sadeh, A., Sharkey, K. M., & Carskadon, M. A. (1994). Activity‐based sleep‐wake identification: An empirical test of methodological issues. Sleep, 17(3), 201–207. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/17.3.201.
      Seehagen, S. (2022). Infant sleep as a cornerstone for cognitive development. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9(1), 104–110. https://doi.org/10.1177/23727322211068006.
      Seehagen, S., Konrad, C., Herbert, J. S., & Schneider, S. (2015). Timely sleep facilitates declarative memory consolidation in infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(5), 1625–1629. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1414000112.
      Simcock, G., Garrity, K., & Barr, R. (2011). The effect of narrative cues on infants' imitation from television and picture books. Child Development, 82(5), 1607–1619. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01636.x.
      Singh, L., Cristia, A., Karasik, L. B., Rajendra, S. J., & Oakes, L. M. (2023). Diversity and representation in infant research: Barriers and bridges toward a globalized science of infant development. Infancy: The Official Journal of the International Society on Infant Studies, 28(4), 708–737. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12545.
      Stickgold, R., & Walker, M. P. (2013). Sleep‐dependent memory triage: Evolving generalization through selective processing. Nature Neuroscience, 16(2), 139–145. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3303.
      Strouse, G. A., & Samson, J. E. (2021). Learning from video: A meta‐analysis of the video deficit in children ages 0 to 6 years. Child Development, 92(1), e20–e38. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13429.
      Strouse, G. A., & Troseth, G. L. (2014). Supporting toddlers' transfer of word learning from video. Cognitive Development, 30, 47–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.01.002.
      Tononi, G., & Cirelli, C. (2006). Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 10(1), 49–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2005.05.002.
      Troseth, G. L., Saylor, M. M., & Archer, A. H. (2006). Young children's use of video as a source of socially relevant information. Child Development, 77(3), 786–799. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00903.x.
      van Dongen, E. V., Thielen, J.‐W., Takashima, A., Barth, M., & Fernández, G. (2012). Sleep supports selective retention of associative memories based on relevance for future utilization. PLoS One, 7(8), e43426. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0043426.
      van Rijn, E., Lucignoli, C., Izura, C., & Blagrove, M. T. (2017). Sleep‐dependent memory consolidation is related to perceived value of learned material. Journal of Sleep Research, 26(3), 302–308. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12457.
      Zack, E., & Barr, R. (2016). The role of interactional quality in learning from touch screens during infancy: Context matters. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, Article 1264. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01264.
      Zack, E., Gerhardstein, P., Meltzoff, A. N., & Barr, R. (2013). 15‐month‐olds' transfer of learning between touch screen and real‐world displays: Language cues and cognitive loads. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 54(1), 20–25. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12001.
    • Grant Information:
      KO 5811/1-1 Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft; SE 2154/6-1 Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft
    • Contributed Indexing:
      Keywords: infants; scaffolding; screen media; sleep‐dependent memory consolidation; transfer deficit
    • Publication Date:
      Date Created: 20231219 Date Completed: 20240715 Latest Revision: 20240715
    • Publication Date:
      20240716
    • Accession Number:
      10.1111/jsr.14121
    • Accession Number:
      38112265