Menu
×
Baxter-Patrick James Island
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 795-6679
West Ashley Library
9 a.m. – 7 p.m.
Phone: (843) 766-6635
Wando Mount Pleasant Library
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 805-6888
Village Library
9 a.m. – 6 p.m.
Phone: (843) 884-9741
St. Paul's/Hollywood Library
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 889-3300
Otranto Road Library
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 572-4094
Mt. Pleasant Library
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 849-6161
McClellanville Library
9 a.m. – 6 p.m.
Phone: (843) 887-3699
Keith Summey North Charleston Library
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 744-2489
John's Island Library
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 559-1945
Hurd/St. Andrews Library
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 766-2546
Folly Beach Library
Closed
Phone: (843) 588-2001
Edisto Island Library
9 a.m. – 6 p.m.
Phone: (843) 869-2355
Dorchester Road Library
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 552-6466
John L. Dart Library
9 a.m. – 7 p.m.
Phone: (843) 722-7550
Main Library
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 805-6930
Bees Ferry West Ashley Library
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 805-6892
Edgar Allan Poe/Sullivan's Island Library
Closed for renovations
Phone: (843) 883-3914
Mobile Library
9 a.m. - 5 p.m.
Phone: (843) 805-6909
Today's Hours
Baxter-Patrick James Island
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 795-6679
West Ashley Library
9 a.m. – 7 p.m.
Phone: (843) 766-6635
Wando Mount Pleasant Library
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 805-6888
Village Library
9 a.m. – 6 p.m.
Phone: (843) 884-9741
St. Paul's/Hollywood Library
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 889-3300
Otranto Road Library
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 572-4094
Mt. Pleasant Library
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 849-6161
McClellanville Library
9 a.m. – 6 p.m.
Phone: (843) 887-3699
Keith Summey North Charleston Library
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 744-2489
John's Island Library
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 559-1945
Hurd/St. Andrews Library
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 766-2546
Folly Beach Library
Closed
Phone: (843) 588-2001
Edisto Island Library
9 a.m. – 6 p.m.
Phone: (843) 869-2355
Dorchester Road Library
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 552-6466
John L. Dart Library
9 a.m. – 7 p.m.
Phone: (843) 722-7550
Main Library
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 805-6930
Bees Ferry West Ashley Library
9 a.m. – 8 p.m.
Phone: (843) 805-6892
Edgar Allan Poe/Sullivan's Island Library
Closed for renovations
Phone: (843) 883-3914
Mobile Library
9 a.m. - 5 p.m.
Phone: (843) 805-6909
Patron Login
menu
Item request has been placed!
×
Item request cannot be made.
×
Processing Request
Interpersonal physiological synchrony is associated with first person and third person subjective assessments of excitement during cooperative joint tasks.
Item request has been placed!
×
Item request cannot be made.
×
Processing Request
- Author(s): Murata A;Murata A; Nomura K; Nomura K; Nomura K; Watanabe J; Watanabe J; Kumano S; Kumano S
- Source:
Scientific reports [Sci Rep] 2021 Jun 15; Vol. 11 (1), pp. 12543. Date of Electronic Publication: 2021 Jun 15.- Publication Type:
Journal Article- Language:
English - Source:
- Additional Information
- Source: Publisher: Nature Publishing Group Country of Publication: England NLM ID: 101563288 Publication Model: Electronic Cited Medium: Internet ISSN: 2045-2322 (Electronic) Linking ISSN: 20452322 NLM ISO Abbreviation: Sci Rep Subsets: MEDLINE
- Publication Information: Original Publication: London : Nature Publishing Group, copyright 2011-
- Subject Terms:
- Abstract: Interpersonal physiological synchrony has been shown to play important roles in social activities. While most studies have shed light on the effects of physiological synchrony on recognition of the group state, such as cohesion or togetherness, the effect of physiological synchrony on the recognition of emotional experience has not been adequately researched. In this study, we examined how physiological synchrony is associated with first- and third-person emotion recognition during a joint task. Two participants played a cooperative block-stacking game (Jenga), alternating their roles as player and adviser, while their heart rates were recorded. The participants evaluated their own emotional experience for each turn. Bystanders watched the game to evaluate the players' emotions. Results showed that the players' subjective excitement increased not only with their own heart rate, but also with increased heart rate synchrony with their adviser. Heart rate synchrony between player and adviser also related to increased intensity in perceived excitement from the bystanders. Given that both first- and third-person emotion recognition can have cumulative impacts on a group, the relationship between physiological synchrony and emotion recognition observed in the present study will help deepen understanding of the psychophysiological mechanisms underlying larger group phenomena such as crowd excitement.
- References: Konvalinka, I. et al. Synchronised arousal between performers and related spectators in a fire-walking ritual. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 108(20), 8514–8519 (2011). (PMID: 21536887310095410.1073/pnas.1016955108)
Liu, N. et al. NIRS-based hyperscanning reveals inter-brain neural synchronization during cooperative Jenga game with face-to-face communication. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10, 82 (2016). (PMID: 27014019478216410.3389/fnhum.2016.00082)
Mitkidis, P., McGraw, J. J., Roepstorff, A. & Wallot, S. Building trust: Heart rate synchrony and arousal during joint action increased by public goods game. Physiol. Behav. 149, 101–106 (2015). (PMID: 2603763510.1016/j.physbeh.2015.05.033)
Gordon. et al. Physiological and behavioral synchrony predict group cohesion and performance. Sci. Rep. 10, 8484 (2020).
Hu, Y. et al. Inter-brain synchrony and cooperation context in interactive decision making. Biol. Psychol. 133, 54–62 (2018). (PMID: 2929223210.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.12.005)
Noy, L., Levit-Binun, N. & Golland, Y. Being in the zone: Physiological markers of togetherness in joint improvisation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9, 187 (2015). (PMID: 25999832441971310.3389/fnhum.2015.00187)
Mønster, D., Håkonsson, D. D., Eskildsen, J. K. & Wallot, S. Physiological evidence of interpersonal dynamics in a cooperative production task. Physiol. Behav. 156, 24–34 (2016). (PMID: 2677346610.1016/j.physbeh.2016.01.004)
Chikersal, P., Tomprou, M., Kim, Y. J., Woolley, A. W., & Dabbish, L. Deep structures of collaboration: Physiological correlates of collective intelligence and group satisfaction. Proceedings of the ACM Conference on CSCW. 873–888 (2017).
Páez, D., Rimé, B., Basabe, N., Wlodarczyk, A. & Zumeta, L. Psychosocial effects of perceived emotional synchrony in collective gatherings. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 108(5), 711 (2015). (PMID: 2582203310.1037/pspi0000014)
Durkheim, É. The Elementary Forms of the Religious Life (Original Work Published 1912) (Oxford University Press, 2008).
Kreibig, S. D. Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biol. Psychol. 84(3), 394–421 (2010). (PMID: 2037137410.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010)
Quadflieg, S. & Penton-Voak, I. S. The emerging science of people-watching: Forming impressions from third-party encounters. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 26(4), 383–389 (2017). (PMID: 10.1177/0963721417694353)
Breithaupt, F. A three-person model of empathy. Emot. Rev. 4(1), 84–91 (2012). (PMID: 10.1177/1754073911421375)
Masuda, T. et al. Placing the face in context: Cultural differences in the perception of facial emotion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 94(3), 365 (2008). (PMID: 1828428710.1037/0022-3514.94.3.365)
Ito, K., Masuda, T., & Man Wai Li, L. Agency and facial emotion judgment in context. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 39(6), 763–776 (2013).
Hess, U., Blaison, C. & Kafetsios, K. Judging facial emotion expressions in context: The influence of culture and self-construal orientation. J. Nonverbal Behav. 40(1), 55–64 (2016). (PMID: 10.1007/s10919-015-0223-7)
Schilbach, L. et al. Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behav. Brain Sci. 36(4), 393–414 (2013). (PMID: 2388374210.1017/S0140525X12000660)
Schilbach, L. et al. Being with virtual others: Neural correlates of social interaction. Neuropsychologia 44(5), 718–730 (2006). (PMID: 1617183310.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.07.017)
Page-Gould, E., Mendoza-Denton, R. & Tropp, L. R. With a little help from my cross-group friend: Reducing anxiety in intergroup contexts through cross-group friendship. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 95(5), 1080 (2008). (PMID: 1895419510.1037/0022-3514.95.5.1080)
Kawaguchi, H., Murakami, B. & Kawai, M. Behavioral characteristics of children with high functioning pervasive developmental disorders during a game. J. Epidemiol. 20, S490–S497 (2010). (PMID: 20179365392040610.2188/jea.JE20090178)
Draghici, A. E. & Taylor, J. A. The physiological basis and measurement of heart rate variability in humans. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 35(1), 22 (2016). (PMID: 27680542503987610.1186/s40101-016-0113-7)
Wilson, S. J. et al. When couples’ hearts beat together: Synchrony in heart rate variability during conflict predicts heightened inflammation throughout the day. Psychoneuroendocrinology 93, 107–116 (2018). (PMID: 29709758600274810.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.04.017)
Bates, D. M., Mäechler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67(1), 1–48 (2015). (PMID: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01)
Torrence, C. & Compo, G. P. A practical guide to wavelet analysis. Bull. Am. Meteorol. Soc. 79(1), 61–78 (1998). (PMID: 10.1175/1520-0477(1998)079<0061:APGTWA>2.0.CO;2)
Grinsted, A., Moore, J. C. & Jevrejeva, S. Application of the cross wavelet transform and wavelet coherence to geophysical time series. Nonlinear Process. Geophys. 11, 561–566 (2004). (PMID: 10.5194/npg-11-561-2004)
Chang, C. & Glover, G. H. Time–frequency dynamics of resting-state brain connectivity measured with fMRI. Neuroimage 50(1), 81–98 (2010). (PMID: 2000671610.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.011)
Hess, U. & Fischer, A. Emotional mimicry: Why and when we mimic emotions. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass. 8(2), 45–57 (2014). (PMID: 10.1111/spc3.12083)
Murata, A., Saito, H., Schug, J., Ogawa, K., & Kameda, T. Spontaneous facial mimicry is enhanced by the goal of inferring emotional states: Evidence for moderation of “automatic” mimicry by higher cognitive processes. PLoS One. 11(4), e0153128 (2016).
Neufeld, J., Ioannou, C., Korb, S., Schilbach, L. & Chakrabarti, B. Spontaneous facial mimicry is modulated by joint attention and autistic traits. Autism Res. 9(7), 781–789 (2016). (PMID: 2644266510.1002/aur.1573)
Chartrand, T. L. & Bargh, J. A. The chameleon effect: The perception–behavior link and social interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 76(6), 893 (1999). (PMID: 1040267910.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893)
Shockley, K., Santana, M. V. & Fowler, C. A. Mutual interpersonal postural constraints are involved in cooperative conversation. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept Perform. 29(2), 326 (2003). (PMID: 1276061810.1037/0096-1523.29.2.326)
Louwerse, M. M., Dale, R., Bard, E. G. & Jeuniaux, P. Behavior matching in multimodal communication is synchronised. Cogn. Sci. 36(8), 1404–1426 (2012). (PMID: 2298479310.1111/j.1551-6709.2012.01269.x)
Emery, N. J. The eyes have it: The neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24(6), 581–604 (2000). (PMID: 1094043610.1016/S0149-7634(00)00025-7)
Tomasello, M. & Carpenter, M. Shared intentionality. Dev. Sci. 10(1), 121–125 (2007). (PMID: 1718170910.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00573.x)
Cañigueral, R. & Hamilton, A. F. C. The role of eye gaze during natural social interactions in typical and autistic people. Front. Psychol. 10, 560 (2019). (PMID: 30930822642874410.3389/fpsyg.2019.00560)
Niedenthal, P. M. Embodying emotion. Science 316(5827), 1002–1005 (2007). (PMID: 1751035810.1126/science.1136930)
Blaesi, S. & Wilson, M. The mirror reflects both ways: Action influences perception of others. Brain Cogn. 72(2), 306–309 (2010). (PMID: 1991476310.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.001)
Marmolejo-Ramos, F. et al. Your face and moves seem happier when I smile. Exp. Psychol. 67(1), 14–22 (2020). (PMID: 3239481410.1027/1618-3169/a000470)
Madwed, J. B., Albrecht, P., Mark, R. G. & Cohen, R. J. Low-frequency oscillations in arterial pressure and heart rate: A simple computer model. Am. J. Physiol. 256(6), H1573–H1579 (1989). (PMID: 2735430)
Pagani, M. et al. Relationship between spectral components of cardiovascular variabilities and direct measures of muscle sympathetic nerve activity in humans. Circulation 95(6), 1441–1448 (1997). (PMID: 911851110.1161/01.CIR.95.6.1441)
Taylor, J. A., Williams, T. D., Seals, D. R. & Davy, K. P. Low-frequency arterial pressure fluctuations do not reflect sympathetic outflow: Gender and age differences. Am. J. Physiol. 274(4), H1194–H1201 (1998). (PMID: 9575922)
James, W. The Principles of Psychology (Henry Holt and Company, 1890).
Laird, J. D. & Lacasse, K. Bodily influences on emotional feelings: Accumulating evidence and extensions of William James’s theory of emotion. Emot. Rev. 6(1), 27–34 (2013). (PMID: 10.1177/1754073913494899)
Jackson, J. C. et al. Synchrony and physiological arousal increase cohesion and cooperation in large naturalistic groups. Sci. Rep. 8(1), 1–8 (2018). (PMID: 10.1038/s41598-017-18023-4)
Chang, A. et al. Body sway reflects joint emotional expression in music ensemble performance. Sci. Rep. 9(1), 1–11 (2019).
Lofland, J. Protest: Studies of Collective Behavior and Social Movements (Transaction Books, 1985).
Antelmi, I. et al. Influence of age, gender, body mass index, and functional capacity on heart rate variability in a cohort of subjects without heart disease. Am. J. Cardiol. 93(3), 381–385 (2004). (PMID: 1475940010.1016/j.amjcard.2003.09.065) - Publication Date: Date Created: 20210616 Date Completed: 20211029 Latest Revision: 20230203
- Publication Date: 20240829
- Accession Number: PMC8206359
- Accession Number: 10.1038/s41598-021-91831-x
- Accession Number: 34131193
- Source:
Contact CCPL
Copyright 2022 Charleston County Public Library Powered By EBSCO Stacks 3.3.0 [350.3] | Staff Login
No Comments.