The effects of warned loss magnitude and timeout duration on human avoidance behavior.

Item request has been placed! ×
Item request cannot be made. ×
loading   Processing Request
  • Author(s): Ono K;Ono K; Koshitsuka Y; Koshitsuka Y; Kubo N; Kubo N
  • Source:
    Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior [J Exp Anal Behav] 2021 Mar; Vol. 115 (2), pp. 481-494. Date of Electronic Publication: 2020 Dec 16.
  • Publication Type:
    Journal Article
  • Language:
    English
  • Additional Information
    • Source:
      Publisher: Wiley-Blackwell Country of Publication: United States NLM ID: 0203727 Publication Model: Print-Electronic Cited Medium: Internet ISSN: 1938-3711 (Electronic) Linking ISSN: 00225002 NLM ISO Abbreviation: J Exp Anal Behav Subsets: MEDLINE
    • Publication Information:
      Publication: [Malden, MA] : Wiley-Blackwell
      Original Publication: Waltham, Mass., Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior.
    • Subject Terms:
    • Abstract:
      This study experimentally investigated the determinants of avoidance behavior when participants are forewarned of aversive outcomes. The effects of 3 variables on avoidance behavior were examined: point-loss amount (5 levels, from 20 to 100 points), duration of timeout from positive reinforcement (5 levels, 20 to 100 s), and 3 predictive accuracy levels (100%, 50%, and 0%) of warning stimuli. Twelve participants completed 3 sessions, each comprising 25 discrete trials, that differed in predictive accuracy level. Throughout a session, a participant engaged in button press responses that were reinforced by points under a conjunctive fixed-ratio fixed-interval schedule. During each trial, a warning stimulus that indicated a loss amount and a timeout duration was presented. If the participant pressed the avoidance button, then the timeout started, otherwise the loss occurred. The trial ended with termination of timeout or an occurrence of the loss. Results showed that avoidance responses increased when the loss amount increased and decreased when the timeout duration increased. The frequency of avoidance responses was lowest when the predictive accuracy of warning stimuli was 0%. These findings demonstrated that this experimental procedure could be useful for investigating human avoidance behavior outside the laboratory.
      (© 2020 Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior.)
    • References:
      Ader, R., & Tatum, R. (1961). Free-operant avoidance conditioning in human subjects. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4(3), 275-276. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1961.4-275.
      Azrin, N. H. (1958). Some effects of noise on human behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1(2), 183-200. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1958.1-183.
      Azrin, N. H., & Holz, W. C. (1966). Punishment. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: Areas of research and application (pp. 380-447). Appleton-Century-Crofts.
      Azrin, N. H., Holz, W. C., & Hake, D. F. (1963). Fixed-ratio punishment. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6(2), 141-148. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1963.6-141.
      Baer, D. M. (1960). Escape and avoidance response of preschool children to two schedules of reinforcement withdrawal. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 3(2), 155-159. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1960.3-155.
      Baron, A. (1991). Avoidance and punishment. In I. H. Iversen & K. A. Lattal (Eds.), Techniques in the behavioral and neural sciences, Vol. 6, Experimental analysis of behavior: Part 1 (pp. 173-217). Elsevier.
      Baron, A., & Galizio, M. (1983). Instructional control of human operant behavior. The Psychological Record, 33(4), 495-520.
      Baron, A., & Kaufman, A. (1966). Human, free-operant avoidance of “time out” from monetary reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9(5), 557-565. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1966.9-557.
      Baron, A., & Kaufman, A. (1968). Facilitation and suppression of human loss-avoidance by signaled, unavoidable loss. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11(2), 177-185. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1968.11-177.
      Brener, J. (1966). Heart rate as an avoidance response. The Psychological Record, 16(3), 329-336.
      Catania, A. C., Matthews, B. A., & Shimoff, E. (1982). Instructed versus shaped human verbal behavior: Interactions with nonverbal responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 38(3), 233-248. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1982.38-233.
      Cherek, D. R., Spiga, R., Steinberg, J. L., & Kelly, T. H. (1990). Human aggressive responses maintained by avoidance or escape from point loss. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53(2), 293-303. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1990.53-293.
      Clark, H. B., Rowbury, T., Baer, A. M., & Baer, D. M. (1973). Timeout as a punishing stimulus in continuous and intermittent schedules. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 6(3), 443-455. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1973.6-443.
      Corralejo, S. M., Jensen, S. A., Greathouse, A. D., & Ward, L. E. (2018). Parameters of time-out: Research update and comparison to parenting programs, books, and online recommendations. Behavior Therapy, 49(1), 99-112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2017.09.005.
      Dardano, J. F. (1965). Modification of observing behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 8(4), 207-214. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1965.8-207.
      DeFulio, A., & Hackenberg, T. D. (2007). Discriminated timeout avoidance in pigeons: The roles of added stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 88(1), 51-71. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2007.59-06.
      Dymond, S. (2019). Overcoming avoidance in anxiety disorders: The contributions of Pavlovian and operant avoidance extinction methods. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 98, 61-70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.01.007.
      Dymond, S., & Roche, B. (2009). A contemporary behavior analysis of anxiety and avoidance. The Behavior Analyst, 32(1), 7-27. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03392173.
      Ferster, C. B. (1958). Control of behavior in chimpanzees and pigeons by time out from positive reinforcement. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 72(8), 1-38. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093787.
      Flanagan, B., Goldiamond, I., & Azrin, N. (1958). Operant stuttering: The control of stuttering behavior through response-contingent consequences. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1(2), 173-177. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1958.1-173.
      Fox, A. E., & Pietras, C. J. (2013). The effects of response-cost punishment on instructional control during a choice task. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 99(3), 346-361. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.20.
      Friman, P. C., & Poling, A. (1995). Making life easier with effort: Basic findings and applied research on response effort. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28(4), 583-590. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1995.28-583.
      Galbicka, G., & Branch, M. N. (1983). Stimulus-food relations and free-operant postponement of timeout from response-independent food presentation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 40(2), 153-163. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1983.40-153.
      Galizio, M. (1979). Contingency-shaped and rule-governed behavior: Instructional control of human loss avoidance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 31(1), 53-70. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1979.31-53.
      Galuska, C. M., Mikorski, J., & Perone, M. (2012). Assessing the role of effort reduction in the reinforcing efficacy of timeout from avoidance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 98(3), 257-271. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2012.98-257.
      Grings, W. W., & Carlin, S. (1966). Instrumental modification of autonomic behavior. The Psychological Record, 16(2), 153-159.
      Harper, J. M., Iwata, B. A., & Camp, E. M. (2013). Assessment and treatment of social avoidance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(1), 147-160. https://doi: https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.18.
      Harris, S. L., & Ersner-Hershfield, R. (1978). Behavioral suppression of seriously disruptive behavior in psychotic and retarded patients: A review of punishment and its alternatives. Psychological Bulletin, 85(6), 1352-1375. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.85.6.1352.
      Hayes, S. C., Brownstein, A. J., Zettle, R. D., Rosenfarb, I., & Korn, Z. (1986). Rule-governed behavior and sensitivity to changing consequences of responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45(3), 237-256. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1986.45-237.
      Higgins, S. T., & Morris, E. K. (1984). Generality of free-operant avoidance conditioning to human behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 96(2), 247-272. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.96.2.247.
      Kaufman, A., Baron, A., & Kopp, R. E. (1966). Some effects of instructions on human operant behavior. Psychonomic Monograph Supplements, 1(11), 243-250.
      Kazdin, A. E. (1973). Time-out for some considerations on punishment. American Psychologist, 28(10), 939-941. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0038232.
      Kelly, J. F., & Hake, D. F. (1970). An extinction-induced increase in an aggressive response with humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 14(2), 153-164. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1970.14-153.
      Lattal, K. A., & Griffin, M. A. (1972). Punishment contrast during free-operant avoidance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 18(3), 509-516. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1972.18-509.
      Leitenberg, H. (1965). Is time-out from positive reinforcement an aversive event? A review of the experimental evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 64(6), 428-441. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0022657.
      Linscheid, T. R., Iwata, B. A., Ricketts, R. W., Williams, D. E., & Griffin, J. C. (1990). Clinical evaluation of the self-injurious behavior inhibiting system (SIBIS). Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23(1), 53-78. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1990.23-53.
      Matthews, B. A., & Shimoff, E. (1974). Human responding on a temporally defined schedule of point-loss avoidance. The Psychological Record, 24(2), 209-219. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03394236.
      Miller, L. K. (1968a). The effect of response force on avoidance rate. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11(6), 809-812. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1968.11-809.
      Miller, L. K. (1968b). Escape from an effortful situation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11(5), 619-627. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1968.11-619.
      Miller, L. K. (1970). Some punishing effects of response-force. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13(2), 215-220. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1970.13-215.
      Morse, W. H., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1956). The maintenance of avoidance behavior using the removal of a conditioned positive reinforcer as the aversive stimulus. American Psychologist, 11(8), 430 (Abstract).
      Rachlin, H., Raineri, A., & Cross, D. (1991). Subjective probability and delay. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55(2), 233-244. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1991.55-233.
      Ruddle, H. V., Bradshaw, C. M., Szabadi, E., & Foster, T. M. (1982). Performance of humans in concurrent avoidance/positive-reinforcement schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 38(1), 51-61. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1982.38-51.
      Schlund, M. W., Treacher, K., Preston, O., Magee, S. K., Richman, D. M., Brewer, A. T., Cameron, G., & Dymond, S. (2017). “Watch out!”: Effects of instructed threat and avoidance on human free-operant approach-avoidance behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 107(1), 101-122. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.238.
      Shipley, C. R., Baron, A., & Kaufman, A. (1972). Effects of timeout from one reinforcer on human behavior maintained by another reinforcer. The Psychological Record, 22(2), 201-210. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03394080.
      Sidman, M. (1953). Avoidance conditioning with brief shock and no exteroceptive warning signal. Science, 118,157-158. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.118.3058.157.
      Sidman, M. (1966). Avoidance behavior. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: Areas of research and application, (pp. 448-498). Prentice-Hall.
      Thomas, J. R. (1965). Discriminated time-out avoidance in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 8(5), 329-338. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1965.8-329.
      Trenholme, I. A., Baron, A., & Kaufman, A. (1969). Effects of signaled time-out from and loss of monetary reinforcement on human operant behavior. Psychonomic Science, 15(6), 295-296. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03336307.
      Weiner, H. (1962). Some effects of response cost upon human operant behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5(2), 201-208. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1962.5-201.
      Weiner, H. (1963). Response cost and the aversive control of human operant behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6(3), 415-421. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1963.6-415.
    • Contributed Indexing:
      Keywords: avoidance; choice; humans; point loss; timeout; warning stimulus
    • Publication Date:
      Date Created: 20201217 Date Completed: 20211028 Latest Revision: 20211028
    • Publication Date:
      20240829
    • Accession Number:
      10.1002/jeab.658
    • Accession Number:
      33331011