Adjective semantics, world knowledge and visual context: comprehension of size terms by 2- to 7-year-old Dutch-speaking children.

Item request has been placed! ×
Item request cannot be made. ×
loading   Processing Request
  • Author(s): Tribushinina E;Tribushinina E
  • Source:
    Journal of psycholinguistic research [J Psycholinguist Res] 2013 Jun; Vol. 42 (3), pp. 205-25.
  • Publication Type:
    Journal Article; Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
  • Language:
    English
  • Additional Information
    • Source:
      Publisher: Springer Country of Publication: United States NLM ID: 0333506 Publication Model: Print Cited Medium: Internet ISSN: 1573-6555 (Electronic) Linking ISSN: 00906905 NLM ISO Abbreviation: J Psycholinguist Res Subsets: MEDLINE
    • Publication Information:
      Publication: Sept. 2004- : New York : Springer
      Original Publication: New York, Plenum Press
    • Subject Terms:
    • Abstract:
      The interpretation of size terms involves constructing contextually-relevant reference points by combining visual cues with knowledge of typical object sizes. This study aims to establish at what age children learn to integrate these two sources of information in the interpretation process and tests comprehension of the Dutch adjectives groot 'big' and klein 'small' by 2- to 7-year-old children. The results demonstrate that there is a gradual increase in the ability to inhibit visual cues and to use world knowledge for interpreting size terms. 2- and 3-year-old children only used the extremes of the perceptual range as reference points. From age four onwards children, like adults, used a cut-off point in the mid-zone of a series. From age five on, children were able to integrate world knowledge and perceptual context. Although 7-year-olds could make subtle distinctions between sizes of various object classes, their performance on incongruent items was not yet adult-like.
    • References:
      Cognition. 1996 Aug;60(2):143-71. (PMID: 8811743)
      Child Dev. 1988 Aug;59(4):888-96. (PMID: 3168627)
      Child Dev. 1989 Aug;60(4):920-32. (PMID: 2758886)
      Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(11):2037-78. (PMID: 16580701)
      Child Dev. 1994 Aug;65(4):1178-92. (PMID: 7956473)
      Mem Cognit. 1975 Nov;3(6):635-47. (PMID: 24203905)
      J Child Lang. 2005 Aug;32(3):535-62. (PMID: 16220634)
      Child Dev. 2004 Mar-Apr;75(2):562-79. (PMID: 15056206)
      Child Dev. 1986 Jun;57(3):583-602. (PMID: 3720394)
      Child Dev. 1980 Dec;51(4):1103-9. (PMID: 7471919)
      Science. 2004 Apr 16;304(5669):438-41. (PMID: 15031438)
      Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(7):2020-32. (PMID: 18355881)
      Acta Psychol (Amst). 2004 Feb-Mar;115(2-3):167-83. (PMID: 14962399)
      Cognition. 2009 Nov;113(2):135-49. (PMID: 19729156)
      Cognition. 1980 Jun;8(2):145-74. (PMID: 7389286)
      Trends Cogn Sci. 2004 Jan;8(1):12-7. (PMID: 14697398)
      Child Dev. 2008 May-Jun;79(3):594-608. (PMID: 18489415)
      Brain Dev. 2003 Apr;25(3):195-9. (PMID: 12689699)
      Psychol Bull. 2008 Jan;134(1):31-60. (PMID: 18193994)
    • Publication Date:
      Date Created: 20120410 Date Completed: 20140106 Latest Revision: 20211021
    • Publication Date:
      20231215
    • Accession Number:
      10.1007/s10936-012-9217-3
    • Accession Number:
      22485023